|
|
Reply
| |
In science, it is often the case that the issue at hand is not as simple as “go ahead, get in a plane, fly around the earth, and see for yourself that the world is not flat.�? Unless two people who disagree on interpretation can agree on an experiment, the results of which would demonstrate who is correct, one may be faced with contradictory interpretations of the same data. A major problem is that many people, including scientists, become fixated on notions of “proof,�?even though this is a concept for mathematics or formal logic, and not science, social science, or just about anything else. I have heard many talk about “proof beyond a reasonable doubt�?in a criminal law context, but this sounds like someone claiming to be “a little bit pregnant.�?
Obviously, we are back to people deciding what constitutes sufficient evidence to justify an action or interpretation, and the term “proof�?seems to be more about making the people who made the decision feel good about themselves than anything else. Instead, all one can do is to make a case for his or her interpretation. In graduate school, I was taught to do this in a specific way; first, one explains exactly what he or she is contending, in as much detail as possible. Then, one can either review the existing interpretations or present one’s evidence. Lastly, one explains exactly why alternative interpretations fail to account for the evidence. And once one is finished, there are only a few options for those who remain skeptical: the critic can attempt to demonstrate that the case presented is one of “splitting hairs,�?that is, there is no significant difference between the old interpretation(s) and the new one being presented to suggest that some sort of “paradigm shift�?is required; the critic can present and argument about how the new interpretation is based upon an inaccurate understanding of the evidence; or the critic can argue that there the issue is still “open,�?because the evidence should be deemed inconclusive.
Unfortunately, few have been trained as I have, and I have been criticized for writing paragraphs that were deemed “too long�?by some or even for obvious “typographical�?errors. But there are other criticisms that are worth mentioning, and the most useful one to discuss here is the presentation of a piece of evidence (or, as is often the case, several abstracts of scientific studies) that appears to contradict a point that I’ve made. I can picture such people smiling smugly as they hit their control-C and control-V keys, thinking “check and mate�?as they click on the “post�?or “submit�?buttons. What almost all of these kinds of people don’t seem to be able to comprehend is that while it may appear from a cursory reading of an abstract that a point is directly refuted, when one examines it closely, it is no such clear demonstration.
An example of this could be the following: let us say that a researcher takes two groups of common lab rats, all of which have been eating a diet considered “normal,�?but one that is undeniably rich in omega 6 PUFAs. One group, considered the “control,�?continues to be fed the same diet, whereas the other group has its fat source changed to fresh coconut oil. After a week or two, the researcher in charge of the experiment notices that some of the rats in the coconut oil group have developed dry spots. Another week goes by and the researcher sacrifices the rats and measures the fatty acids in the rats�?bodies. He or she concludes that because the rats that developed the dry spots did not have omega 6 PUFAs in their bodies that this is a clear demonstration of “essential fatty acid deficiency.�? While this could indeed be the case, when one examines the evidence as a whole it is clear that this is a totally unreasonable view of the results.
For one thing, rats were fed a totally fat-free diet in experiments done in 1948, and no problems were observed. Another important point is that if one’s diet is changed significantly, short-term effects such as the dry spots is not uncommon, and so the experiment should have lasted longer, until the rats died of natural causes (which would allow us to know about the mortality associated with specific kinds of diets). Moreover, if one does have arachidonic acid (AA) in one’s cells, as the rats would have had at the outset of the experiment, changing to a diet with only coconut oil as a source of fat would result in AA “liberation,�?which for a short period of time could cause various symptoms, unless the rats were fed a diet high in antioxidant-rich foods that they would have access to in the wild.
Unlike history, science is often contextual (in history, one can only assess what actually did happen, and setting up control groups is not possible). However scientists seem to be largely unaware of this point, even though there is the famous example of Newton's "laws of motions," which work well for us humans, but do not under conditions that we could not live under, as Einstein showed. In today's dietary studies, it is almost always assumed, whether or not the researchers are aware of it, that the subjects are consuming a diet rich in omega 6 PUFAs, and thus the findings are contextual, that is, they apply to people on such diets, but not necessarily to those, like myself, who are consuming a diet that is very different. As I learned in graduate school, it is unacceptable to find one piece of evidence that appears to suggest that a particular interpretation is inaccurate, cite it, and then ignore everything else. Evidence must be examined in a comprehensive way, and this appears to be something many if not most scientists have great difficulty doing, perhaps because there is so much “specialization�?today.
|
|
First
Previous
2-3 of 3
Next
Last
|
Reply
| 0 recommendations | Message 2 of 3 in Discussion |
|
This message has been deleted due to termination of membership. |
|
Reply
| |
I considered rejecting your post at first, because you did not provide one example of the practice you find so detestable or misleading. However, I can't imagine what you're talking about, so I'll allow others to judge for themselves, assuming they have any interest in the use of quotation marks at all. I will point out the following: I usually use the word QUOTE if there are statements quoted in the passage, though I could use ' along with ". If there is no statement quoted in the passage I quote, then I sometimes just use " but other times I use the QUOTE/UNQUOTE format. In any case, I don't see why anyone would think these practices are distracting or confusing. My focus is on being clear and concise, and I don't care what others think in that context, to be blunt. This is why, for example, I will sometimes say "and/or" even though "or" is supposed to suffice. I try to leave no room for ambiguity, even if it is a practice considered somehow "wrong" by some great "expert." |
|
|