I was watching a TV show about a man with amnesia who was convicted of vehicular homicide (or something similar). The problem was that he was the passenger, not the driver. He "remembered" incorrectly, and confessed. The evidence in the vehicle showed that he was on the passenger side during the collision, and the first eyewitness on the scene, a person who lived nearby and had no reason to lie, also said that he was the passenger, not the driver (this evidence was not presented at the trial). Eventually, a court ordered that he be given a new trial, and the prosecutor offered him time served to plead guilty, which is what he did.
In science today, if you detect a flaw in a claim, there is no "appeals process," unless you are one of the few people who have "credentials." But even then, there is a good chance that you will do little more than "wreck your career." See the article that I cut and pasted passages from below. In a sense, there is nothing to appeal. The evidence is what it is, and the interpretation is the critical element, at least in nutrition. I'm still surprised at how terribly wrong so much "expert advice" is these days, in the sense that there is direct contradictory evidence, and in science that is supposed to be a refutation, not a signal to become more vehement in one's support for the discredited notion. What I'm trying to do on this site is to supply the necessary analysis tools to people who want to read the evidence for themselves and come to their own conclusions, which is now possible because of its availability on the internet.
QUOTE: Of some 69 letters from readers that have been published in The Scientist since our format and editorial changes of last May, over 40% deal with just three subjects: the difficulty of reconciling religion and science (prompted by William Provine’s provocative opinion piece published in our September 5 edi- tion, page 10); the issue of whether to accept rebel or “heretic�?scientists who espouse minority views; and the inadequacies of peer review...
Rebellious scientists made up the second most popular category among those who sent us letters. Harold Hillman of the University of Surrey, U.K., believes that ex- perimental results based on scan- ning tunneling microscopy contain significant artifacts that distort the data. Peter Duesberg of the Univer- sity of California, Berkeley, holds the view that AIDS is not necessarily caused by a virus. Just as controver- sial, perhaps more, is the claim of Jacques Benveniste and his col- leagues of INSERM, Clamart, France, that highly diluted solutions have a “memory.�?The vehemence with which these views, and these scientists, have been condemned by their colleagues has created a stir in the general media as well. Other scientists who have made similar waves are NIH’s Walter Stewart and Ned Feder—in the sense that their activities and conclusions about the extent of fraud in science have not been well received by their fellow professionals. Science’s intolerance—if it is that-for minority views also seems to be evident in the third most writ- ten-about subject: peer review. As I have had occasion to observe in the past, there is great dissatisfaction with peer review throughout the scientific community, and especial- ly with blind reviewing and the lack of recourse a scientist has if his or her work is reviewed unfairly. What all three of these topics share is a concern with the stand- ards, professional and ethical, that the scientific community takes an interest in—matters distinct from the process of scientific investiga- tion itself. I believe that this interest refutes the common perception that the scientific endeavor is a value- free enterprise. In fact, like all other human endeavors, science is replete with expressions of value—moral and ethical... UNQUOTE.
Source: THE SCIENTIST @ 2(24):10,26 December 1988.
On the internet: http://209.85.165.104/search?q=cache:OlPXBxOovEgJ:www.garfield.library.upenn.edu /essays/v14p326y1991.pdf+%22harold+hillman%22+%22peter+duesberg%22&hl= en&ct=clnk&cd=2&gl=us |